In Tomasello’s book, “The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition”, we learn that all species are designed to function within a particular environment. More specifically, human infants and young children require exposure to a particular kind of social environment or culture to support their social and cognitive development. Parents need to raise their children in a nurturing environment in order to facilitate their cultural learning. Human infants and young children are born completely dependent on their parents. A child’s experiences will be based solely on the practices of the family they are born into.
Tomasello (1999) looks at the way in which these human cultural environments shape our children’s cognitive development: The particular habitus into which a child is born determines the kinds of social innteractions she will have, the kinds of physical objects she will have available, the kinds of learning experiences and opportunities she will encounter, and the kinds of inferences she will draw about the way of life of those around her. The habitus thus has direct effects on cognitive development in terms of the “raw material” with which the child has to work. (p.79)
This concept led me to think about the children who are deprived of these “raw materials” and not raised in a nurturing environment conducive to their cultural learning. I was curious to learn more about the severe cases if neglect seen in feral or isolated children. I was baffled that these children could survive in such horrific conditions and in disbelief that their own parents were the ones who not only denied their own babies the basic necessities of life but the love and affection every child deserves. What impact did this social isolation have on their cognitive development and what was their capacity for learning after they were found and introduced to a world they have not had the opportunity to learn from?
During my research, I came across a very interesting concept proposed by Eric Lenneberg (1967) called the “Critical Period Hypothesis”. This hypothesis states “there are maturational constraints on the time a first language can be acquired. First language acquisition relies on neuroplasticity. If language acquisition does not occur by puberty (around age 12), some aspects of language can be learnt but full mastery cannot be achieved” (p. 180). Because this hypothesis cannot be ethically tested on humans and animals lack the capacity to acquire language, cases of feral or isolated children have been studied to support or refute this hypothesis
I would like to introduce you to two of the most well-known and documented cases of feral/isolated children.
GENIE
Genie was born Susan Wiley in Arcadia, California in 1957. Her father suffering from depression decided that she was retarded. He felt he needed to “protect” her from the outside world so he kept her locked up in a dark room strapped to a potty chair until for most of her life. She was discovered at the age of 13 in diapers barely able to walk or talk, and unable to chew or swallow food. She was found to have a vocabulary of about 20 words, most of which were negative, leading scientists to believe she was abused for making noise but despite making some progress in acquiring some new words she was never able to function as a “normal” adult.
Video: "Secret of the Wild Child" Part 1 DANI |
Dani was born Danielle Crockett in 1998 and lived with her mother in Florida. Despite numerous attempts from neighbours calling the child abuse hotline to report about the “The Girl in the Window” who has never been seen outside of her home, it took three years before a case worker and police officer came to the house. In July of 2005, Danielle was found locked in a filthy, roach infested room curled up on a soiled mattress in an overflowing diaper, her body full of insect bites and sores. She was severely malnourished, unable to speak or make eye contact, and was found to have the mental and physical capacities of an infant. Danielle was taken to hospital and put in foster care and adopted by a loving family 2 years later. Her new adoptive family called her Dani. With an incredible amount of love and support from her family and workers she continues to show progress. While it has been reported that Danielle will never be a “normal child” she is now responsive to affection, riding horses, swimming, walking, communicating with non-verbal cues, and doing Pre-K work in school.
Article printed in 2005 when she was discovered:
“The Girl in the Window” You tube video
Dani with her new adoptive parents, Bernie and Diane Lierow, and her stepbrother, William
Check out Dani's Blog to see the progress she has made:
While it is unclear the extent to which their abusive upbringing might have contributed to their severely impaired cognitive function, I still think these cases support the “critical period hypothesis” through their inability to learn the language skills considered “normal” for their age group. Aside from their ability to learn language, I found it very interesting to compare their overall progress based on their environments they were exposed to after they were found. I think Dani’s adoption into a loving, supportive, and culturally rich learning environment definitely contributed to her progress while Genie was the object of experiments for years and lived in adult foster homes. Genie was making some
progress but her environment caused her to regress again when she was abused in one of the foster homes and she never found her way back. It is amazing to see the progress Dani is making and I will continue beyond this post to stay tuned into her own blog site I posted earlier.
References Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press. Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological Foundations of Language. New York: Wiley. |
Hi Tara,
ReplyDeleteThanks so much for bringing this unique perspective to our collective learning process. Your blog totally pulled at my heartstrings, from the extreme stories, but also because my family was recently formed through adoption. My partner and I adopted three kids this past year- 8, 4, and 1. They are all siblings from Dartmouth, from Family and Children's Services.
While I wholeheartedly place my bets in the nurture side of the eternal nature versus nurture debate, the "critical period hypothesis" is quite an interesting one, and something I may do more research about.
I am amazed every day by how resilient my own kids are, even though they have experienced neglect and trauma. A supportive, loving, and "forever" environment goes a long way to create the stability required so they can explore the boundaries of their capabilities, develop new skills and language.
Certainly our nine-year-old needs a lot of speech therapy, and we're hoping that's not the case for our one-year-old. But, on the other hand, our nine-year-old has the spirit of a race horse out to conquer the field. Love and nurturing- a prerequisite for learning, that's my theory.
Tara, I would like to extend sincere thanks for your blog on severely neglected children. Reading your blog further fueled my growing curiosity on the subject of feral or isolated children, so much so that it inspired me to continue the discussion in my second blog. Perhaps this is partly the intention behind blog sharing – reading others’ interpretations and ideas to create new interests and provide new perspectives to one’s own research and understanding. Thanks, in part, to the glimpse you provided into the lives of two isolated children, Genie and Dani, I felt compelled to do more research on this rare phenomenon. I found Lenneberg’s thoughts on acquiring language to also be of particular interest. Studying the acquisition of language in feral or isolated children provides practical insight into how we, as humans, can achieve the ability to communicate with language even long after our formative years. It is clear that an isolated child will not benefit from the cultural emergence experienced by most. However, even without the initial acquisition of language or after losing this ability due to separation from society, hope is not lost. Though someone who acquires language skills later in life may not demonstrate full mastery of their language, they can learn to communicate.
ReplyDeleteHey Tara,
ReplyDeleteHow did I miss this blog?! Really. I love hearing about interesting (and horrifying) cases of children whose develpment didn't go "normally". Wondering about all the factors that may have contributed is fascinating to me, and there's certainly no way of knowing what they all were. However, like you, I can see how these cases seem to support the "critical period hypothesis" proposed by Linnenberg.
Having a psychological background helped me understand that very concept, as the maleability of our brains seem to decrease with age- specifically around the age of 12, as also mentionned by Linnenberg.
Linking it to the lack of "raw materials" as proposed by Tomasello seems appropriate and complimentary, too. I love when theories work together.
Mau